Paper for PATT-15 Haarlem 2005
A case study of the inter-relationship between Science
and Technology: England 1984-2004
Frank Banks and Prof. Bob McCormick
R
esearch Group for International Research and Development inT
Faculty of Education and Language Studies
Floor 3, Stuart Hall Building
The Open University
Walton Hall
Milton Keynes
MK7 6AA
United Kingdom
Contact: Frank Banks
Tel: +44 1908 654122
E-mail:
eacher Education across Cultures and Societies (RITES)f.banks@open.ac.uk2
A case study of the inter-relationship between Science
and Technology: England 1984-2004
Abstract
This paper takes the case of England over the twenty years 1984 to 2004 and explores the
relationships between the two linked, but separate, school subjects of science and
technology. Using a framework of analysis, we consider three inter-related strands: the
curriculum rationale (focusing on the specified curriculum); teacher knowledge (focusing
on the enacted curriculum); and pupil learning (focusing on the experienced curriculum).
We consider the strands as they apply to each subject separately and also in relation to
each other and, for each strand, attempt to draw lessons that can be learned and applied to
other national contexts.
Key Words: Science and Technology; Teacher Knowledge; Pupil Learning; Curriculum
Development.
Introduction
Politicians often refer to Science and Technology as an epistemological unit. In the early
1980s, the UK Thatcher Government generously financed a Technical and Vocational
Education Initiative (TVEI) which tried to explicitly bring together the two curriculum
areas of Science and Technology.
the teaching of the two subject areas, which stresses the differences, there are some clear
similarities. Both subjects opened up the group of pupils who would take the subject
(science moved from being a specialist subject just for those going on to do it at higher
levels, Technology had to appeal to the academically ‘able’ as well as the traditional
group of ‘non-academic’ pupils), both make much of ‘hands-on’ learning; both claim to
promote problem solving and other ‘processes’; both try to explicitly link school tasks to
useful learning for every day life and the needs of the work-place.
Our framework of analysis is illustrated graphically (Figure 1).
science and technology developments in England over the last twenty years, both
separately and together through their common features, by considering three strands:
1 Indeed, despite the school politics that has surrounded2 We consider both•
documents, which, in England, has statutory significance);
Curriculum rationale (the specified curriculum as found in national curriculum•
bear to plan and implement their teaching);
teacher knowledge (focusing on the enacted curriculum, i.e. what they bring to1
applied science (see McCormick, 1990; McCulloch, Jenkins & Layton, 1985).
There have of course been a number of moves to link these two, most notoriously the failed attempts at2
Assessment.
This framework was developed for an Open University course E836 Learning, Curriculum and3
Science
Time
Technology
Curriculum Rationale (Specified)
Teacher Knowledge (Enacted)
Pupil Learning (Experienced)
1984
2004
•
above are interpreted and made sense of by pupils).
Through our research at the Open University in both science and technology school
lessons, we explore common issues and consider what each subject can learn from the
other.
countries.
pupil learning (focusing on the experienced curriculum, i.e. how both of the3 We hope that the case of England will highlight issues for consideration in otherFigure 1. Analysis Framework
3
research upon which we draw, and whose contributions have helped form our views.
We would like to acknowledge our debt to the teachers and colleagues who have been involved in the4
The Specified Curriculum
The following statements are from the current National Curriculum in England published
in 1999:
The importance of science
Science stimulates and excites pupils’ curiosity about phenomena and events in the world around them.
It also satisfies this curiosity with knowledge. Because science links direct practical experience with
ideas, it can engage learners at many levels. Scientific method is about developing and evaluating
explanations through experimental evidence and modelling. This is a spur to critical and creative
thought. Through science, pupils understand how major scientific ideas contribute to technological
change — impacting on industry, business and medicine and improving quality of life. Pupils
recognise the cultural significance of science and trace its worldwide development. They learn to
question and discuss science-based issues that may affect their own lives, the direction of society and
the future of the world.
The importance of design and technology
Design and technology prepares pupils to participate in tomorrow’s rapidly changing technologies.
They learn to think and intervene creatively to improve quality of life. The subject calls for pupils to
become autonomous and creative problem solvers, as individuals and members of a team. They must
looks for needs, wants and opportunities and respond to them by developing a range of ideas and
making products and systems. They combine practical skills with an understanding of aesthetics,
social and environmental issues, function and industrial practices. As they do so, they reflect on and
evaluate present and past design and technology, its uses and effects. Through design and technology,
all pupils can become discriminating and informed users of products, and become innovators.
(DfES/QCA, 1999)
These two statements lay out what has been the culmination of a change process in the
school curricula of science and technology (in England ‘Design and Technology’) over
the last twenty years, namely the rationale for the designation of the two subjects as
required areas of study during the years of compulsory schooling, 5-16. In 2005 the
requirement for all pupils to study technology is restricted, and Design and Technology
(D&T) is now only an obligatory subject between the ages of 5-14 years. Science,
however, is still a requirement for all pupils up to 16 years of age. Over the last twenty
years, what lessons can be drawn about making the two subjects compulsory for all
pupils? What decisions were taken about what all should be able to ‘know, understand
and do’ as a result of studying science and technology in the school curriculum and what
was communicated to teachers, pupils and their parents?
Science led the way. Building on the curriculum initiatives of the 1960s following the
‘Sputnik panic’, by the 1980s most secondary schools required all pupils to learn science
and, as most schools were becoming ‘comprehensive’ (non selective), there was a desire
to widen the science curriculum to all pupils including those less academically gifted.
For example, Nuffield Secondary Science became popular, as did Science at Work in the
1970s as curricula specifically designed for such students. Science in England was being
accepted as a core area of study for all pupils on a similar footing to mathematics and
(mother-tongue) English. Also in the early 1980s the nature of a science curriculum for
all pupils – a science for citizenship or ‘scientific literacy’ was being debated. In
particular, the importance of learning ‘facts’ in science was questioned and a case was
made that the processes of the scientific method were much more important for all pupils.
The government policy document Science 5-16 (DES,1985), that pre-dated the national
5
curriculum, did not merely define what should be taught in terms of content such as
‘electricity’ or ‘plants’, but instead emphasised the importance of a process approach.
Indeed, science curriculum innovation in the middle 1980s saw a large number of new
courses such as ‘Warwick Process Science’ and ‘Science in Process’ for secondary
schools. These focused, not on science concepts, but rather on processes such as
observation, interpretation and classification — aspects critical to ‘the scientific method’.
This mood was picked up in the developing primary science curriculum at that time.
Although not totally accepted by some (for example, Jenkins, 1987), many in the
teaching profession generally welcomed a move away from what was often considered as
merely the memorising of poorly understood facts. In contrast, there emerged a generally
common consensus that science might be more accessible to all pupils if it emphasised
skills applicable to other areas of life both inside and outside school. The attention to
‘doing’ science — raising questions that could be answered by an investigation —
became the corner-stone of the developing primary science and in 2005 is a core principle
in new courses which pick up on perceived failures of the national curriculum (see
below). For example, the question ‘What is the best carrier bag?’ would be turned into an
investigation question such as ‘Which carrier bag carries the greatest weight?’ in what
was considered a problem-solving approach. To answer such a question, so-called
‘dependent and independent’ variables were identified. Thus the importance of the
procedural knowledge of science was developed. At this time (1980s), primary teachers
(normally untrained in science) were concerned about the introduction of science into
their day to day work. The rhetoric from those advocating that science should indeed be
part of the primary curriculum was that the teachers could ‘learn with the pupils’; it was
argued that only the process was important, not the science facts or concepts that the
teacher did or did not know.
At this time there was concern more generally in schools in England about what was
needed as a preparation for adult life. Education for competency in the work-place and
the need to be able to ‘problem solve’ was seen as essential. Those advocating a core
place for both science and technology in the curriculum of all pupils used the fact that
problem-solving lay at the heart of the subjects as a key part of the argument.
a different slant to the procedural knowledge, with the implication that general problemsolving
processes could be identified. As we shall argue later, however, this latter
assumption was erroneous. Also, we shall see that problem-solving in science is different
to that in technology. However, despite the push to introduce primary science in the
1970s (e.g. Science 5-13) little had been achieved and, in most schools, Primary science
is a relatively recent development. Just twenty years ago, Harlen could write a book
entitled
science was then being taught in primary schools.
There was something of a backlash to the ‘process is all that is important’ line and the
debate became heated (see Millar & Driver, 1987; Millar, 1988; Screen, 1988;
Wellington, 1988 & 1989; Woolnough, 1988
‘observation’ in isolation for the sake of it was pointless - one had to apply the process to
4 This gavePrimary Science: Taking the Plunge (Harlen, 1985) reflecting the fact that little). Some argued that, for example,4
See Murphy et al. (1995) for the tracing of this for technology and Garrett (1987) for science.6
the understanding of science concepts.
initiatives emphasising constructivist learning ideas, resulting in a concern for pupil
conceptual development and the associated pedagogy. The purpose of the science
curriculum as the acquisition of ‘facts’ was, however, very deep-rooted. The National
Curriculum for Science was first published in 1988 and, although it had an area devoted
to process issues, was largely a re-emphasis on teaching ‘content’ or conceptual
knowledge. The balance had shifted again away from procedural knowledge, reinforced
at all levels by national testing which, despite the rhetoric of a concern for understanding
concepts, emphasised memory and did not include a practical element. In the rapid
revisions of the science curriculum over the last 15 years, the push has been to cut back
on the extent of content in the curriculum but the premier position of scientific method in
the curriculum would never be repeated. Throughout the period we have the shift in
concern and balance of procedural and conceptual knowledge, a theme which is reflected
in different ways in technology as a curriculum subject.
Technology is a relative newcomer to the curriculum for all pupils from 5 to 16 years.
The compulsory National Curriculum was introduced in 1990 and focused on
Technology as a process concerning design. It had four attainment targets:
5 This was also accompanied by research-led•
Attainment target 1 – Identifying needs and opportunities•
Attainment target 2 – Generating a design•
Attainment target 3 – Planning and making•
This process-based curriculum was difficult to implement for both secondary and primary
schools. Primary teachers were unused to considering designing, although craft activities
had long been a feature of primary school life. It was also suggested that a wide range of
teachers become involved at secondary level to cover material areas such as food and
textiles, and aspects of business studies as well as the more traditional materials of wood,
metal and plastics. Few secondary teachers could bring practical experience of design in
the way they did for skills and craft work.
After only two years, The Engineering Council produced a damning report by Smithers
and Robinson which declared that ‘Technology in the National Curriculum is a mess’
(Smithers & Robinson, 1992, p 1). Their main criticism was that by defining technology
solely through a process approach meant that almost all problem-solving activity could be
considered as ‘technology’
Attainment target 4 – EvaluatingDefined on problem-solving alone, most activities become technology - writing this report, conducting
a scientific experiment, finding one's way to a railway station. What is needed is some statement of
technology's domain. (Smithers & Robinson, 1992, p 3).
The report made recommendations as to what should be considered the subject domain of
technology and what should not, and for a better balance between process and content. It
5
Science Project (Foulds, Gott & Mashiter, 1990), the Children’s Leaning in Science Project (CLISP)
Driver, and Oldham (1986) and the primary focused SPACE project (Liverpool University, 1994).
The concern for more refined views of ‘scientific method’ led to a number of projects: The Exploration of7
also tried to untangle the ‘vocational’ and ‘basic skills’ labels that some had attached to
the new compulsory subject, and it advocated a consideration of the ‘literature’ of
technology; looking at and learning from the products and artifacts that already exist
which can inform designing and making. Subsequent developments tried to address these
concerns. In 1995 a new version of the curriculum for England and Wales gave a clearer
steer to what D&T was, and the main activities that should be employed:
Pupils should be given opportunities to develop their design & technology capability through:
Assignments in which they design and make products, focussing on different contexts and
materials and making use of:
Resistant materials;
Compliant materials and/or food (DMAs- Design and Make Assignments).
Focused practical tasks (FPTs) in which they develop and practise particular skills and knowledge;
Activities in which they Investigate, Disassemble and Evaluate familiar products and
Applications. (IDEAS) (DFE/WO, 1995, p. 6)
This methodology strongly reflected the pedagogic model promoted by Nuffield Design
and Technology (Barlex
had looked for progress in each part of a process, Designing and Making and, eventually,
considering even this separation as unhelpful to, now, just one attainment target Design
and Making. Although there is a better balance of knowledge, skills and elements of the
design process, the current Attainment Target is still based around the design process,
with pupils being expected to achieve the following at the penultimate level:
et al 1994). There was a reduction to two attainment targets thatPupils use a wide range of appropriate sources of information to develop ideas. They investigate form,
function and production processes before communicating ideas, using a variety of media. They
recognise the different needs of a range of users and develop fully realistic designs. They produce
plans that predict the time needed to carry out the main stages of making products. They work with a
range of tools, materials, equipment, components and processes, taking full account of their
characteristics. They adapt their methods of manufacture to changing circumstances, providing a sound
explanation for any change from the design proposal. They select appropriate techniques to evaluate
how their products would perform when used and modify their products in the light of the evaluation to
improve their performance.
(QCA http://www.nc.uk.net/)
Unlike science, D&T has tended not to use concepts to organise content, and in areas
where conceptual knowledge is important, for example, control and electronics, this can
be a problem as we shall show later (McCormick, 1997 & 2004; Murphy
D&T, although a required subject for all pupils under the 1990 national curriculum, was
always under attack from those who could not see the justification for that position. The
reasons for the animosity range from those who would put science and technology
together as one curriculum domain (especially at primary school level) to those who,
more prosaically, just considered the subject too expensive to deliver in terms of tools,
equipment and materials. The response from the D&T lobby was to argue that the
subject was important as it prepared ‘pupils to participate in tomorrow’s rapidly changing
technologies’ and so much was done to introduce new technologies such as CAD/CAM
mainly at secondary level as a tool for the designing and making processes.
8
So, in both Science and technology, there has been debate over the last twenty years as to
the balance that should exist in the specified curriculum in both subjects between
procedural knowledge and conceptual knowledge. However, these debates have largely
been within each subject community, independent of each other, and tend to emphasise
the inevitable differences between the goals of each subject rather than the common
ground between the subjects. What can be learned generally, and what can the two
subjects learn from each other?
A key lesson to be learned by the rapid revisions of the specified curriculum of both
science and technology in England over the last twenty years is that it is very difficult to
impose a curriculum onto teachers. As will become evident, a top-down method of
seeking to describe the curriculum in close detail without working with teachers, and
those involved in pre-service and in-service teacher education, to develop a common
understanding of purpose, leads to a mismatch between a teacher’s ‘personal subject
construct’ and what is prescribed to be taught. Teachers have a view about what their
subject is about and, although they wish their pupils to do well in externally set
examinations, when the specified curriculum moves independently of these held views
teachers feel obliged to ‘teach to the tests’. It is therefore imperative that the tests
accurately reflect the intentions of the curriculum designers.
et al, 2004).6As evidence of the state of science, in 2002, the Westminster Parliament Science and
Technology Committee reported on
following as part of the document summary:
Science education from 14 to 19 and said theScience has been a core part of the education of all students up to age of 16 since the introduction of
the National Curriculum in 1989. Most students take double science GCSE [the national examination
syllabus] from 14 to 16. This course aims to provide a general science education for all and, at the
same time, to inspire and prepare some for science post-16. It does neither of these well. It may not be
possible for a single course to fulfil both these needs. Government is supporting a pilot that may be
resolve these tensions, which is welcome but not enough. Existing GCSE courses should be changed
and a wider range of options in science offered to students. […]
Current GCSE courses are overloaded with factual content, contain little contemporary science and
have stultifying assessment arrangements. Coursework is boring and pointless. Teachers and students
are frustrated by the lack of flexibility. Students lose any enthusiasm that they once had for science.
Those that choose to continue with science post-16 often do so in spite of their experiences of GCSE
rather than because of them. Primary responsibility should lie with the awarding bodies; the approach
to assessment at GCSE discourages good science from being taught in schools.
(House of Commons, 2002, p. 5)
The national assessment for D&T also has its critics:
teachers provide coaching which allows pupils to pass through the assessment hoops for D&T GCSE
coursework at the expense of following the wider rationale of D&T learning objectives
(OfSTED, 2000, p3)
6
what he and his colleagues thought were assessing what the curriculum was aiming for.
Kimbell (1997) gives an account of the failure of the government to produce tests in D&T that reflected9
….public examinations in D&T have, one the one hand, enabled many pupils to achieve success in
terms of performance, whilst on the other hand, they have wasted valuable education opportunities for
the development of high order thinking skills at a crucial stage in a pupil’s education.
(Atkinson, 2000, p277)
In response to these criticisms the science community has attempted to introduce courses
which exploit the relevance of science to contemporary life. Courses being piloted in
2005 include ‘Science for the 21
providing science literacy for all and a grounding in science basics for those who wish to
study the subject further. D&T has for a long time taken refuge in the links to ‘real life’
and has tried to provide pupils with authentic tasks. However, as the quotations indicate,
to make the real world manageable to pupils within the constraints of time and resources
that schools impose on the participants sometime leads to an algorithmic approach to the
processes – going through the motions in a mechanistic way -and merely showing a
‘veneer of achievement’ (McCormick
The last twenty years has seen extremes in both science and technology education. Tasks
in technology, such as building and testing various model bridges to destruction, at the
one extreme, to lock-step production of a textile bag (to take home) where the only
design decisions concern the decoration, at the other. In science, tasks have ranged from
making twenty observations on a burning candle to open-ended investigations on
conditions for plant growth to memorizing the names of the parts of a flower.
So the government’s attempts to control what pupils learn by specifying in detail the
curriculum has had limited success according to those charged with monitoring its
impact. We turn now to consider wider lessons to be learned from how the curriculum is
enacted by teachers.
st Century’, which attempts to ‘square the circle’ ofet al, 1994)The Enacted Curriculum
To explore the nature of the ‘enacted’ curriculum, we draw on classroom research we
have conducted in both science and technology lessons. We highlight two aspects of our
research which gives some insight into the problems teachers in England have faced in
trying to enact the specified curriculum. The first considers the teaching of problem
solving in science and technology, the second centers on teacher professional knowledge
and in particular its implications for the teacher training curriculum. Both examples,
however, highlight that the way that teachers enact the specified curriculum depends on
their own professional knowledge.
Teacher professional Knowledge
In our observation of teaching it is evident that success or failure of lessons organised by
teachers was often linked, not only to their college-based subject knowledge and their
choice of pedagogic strategies, but also to their appreciation of how their subject is
transformed into a school subject. In D&T, in particular, an appreciation of the way the
subject in schools had been created by an amalgam of the requirements of a national
curriculum, the personal history of the teachers who currently teach this ‘new’ subject
and the contextual constraints of accommodation, materials and equipment conspire
together to create a particular area of teacher knowledge. We call this ‘school
10
knowledge’. Working with colleagues in Finland, Canada, New Zealand and other areas
of the UK we have seen that the key areas of teacher knowledge: Subject knowledge,
Pedagogic knowledge and School knowledge can provide a framework for us to consider
teacher expertise in a number of different national contexts (see Banks
However, as indicated above there is more to consider than what is required by the state
and the teaching capability of the teacher. Lying at the heart of the dynamic process
between the different aspects of teacher knowledge are the ‘personal subject constructs’
of the teacher, a complex amalgam of past knowledge, experiences of learning, a personal
view of what constitutes ‘good’ teaching and how pupils learn, and belief in the purposes
of the subject. This all underpins a teacher's professional knowledge and is as relevant
for highly experienced teacher as it is for the novice. A student teacher needs to question
his or her personal beliefs about their subject as they work out a rationale for their
classroom practice. But so must those teachers who, although more expert, have
experienced profound changes of what contributes 'school knowledge' during their career
(as has happened with the introduction of the national curriculum in England),
particularly when that knowledge is open to external scrutiny by Her Majesty’s
Inspectors of Schools.
et al, 2004).Example: science tasks and technological contexts
This example of the enacted curriculum draws on our work on the implementation of
science tasks in the classroom (See McCormick
1997). Models of science investigation are depicted as problem-solving processes (e.g.
Gott and Murphy, 1987). Although such models are not simplistic step-like processes,
they are interpreted as such; just as design processes are in technology education.
Primary teachers of science now often use planning sheets and indeed are advised to use
them to support children's procedural decision making. These sheets identify stages in
children's decision making and ask them to focus on specific features e.g. what shall I do
to make it fair, what shall I measure, how shall I measure? This produces in the child's
mind a notion that these questions and procedures are appropriate and useful across
et al, 1996; Murphy & McCormickallproblems.
In Secondary school science there is ritual that has grown out of what constitutes a
'tradition' of procedure in practical science. For example, it was common practice, and
often remains so, to structure reports of experimental activity around
title, method, resultsand
'What I did to make it fair' prior to the method section or directly following it in the
report. In observations in classrooms, selected because of 'good' practice in science,
pupils were found to be including a whole range of disparate procedures under 'fairness'.
These included the setting up of the test of the independent variable as fairness was
translated to mean 'sameness' hence 'I tested X then Y then Z', 'I did the same thing', was
an aspect of fairness in the pupil’s mind. The ‘assessed practical’ is an example of ritual
in science being supported by the concern of teachers for pupils to do well in the external
examination regime in England. Hooke’s Law, the observation that within the elastic
limit, extension of a stretched spring being proportional to the load applied, is a common
practical investigation easily carried out and understood by 11 year old pupils. However,
as this is a phenomenon which many 15 year old pupils can offer an hypothesis which
11
can be investigated, for which results can be quickly and easily gathered and data
graphically displayed, it is often repeated for assessment purposes. This is an example of
the dead-hand of assessment criticized as ‘boring and pointless’ by the parliamentary
committee in the quotation earlier.
The authenticity of science tasks can also be thwarted even when teachers attempt to
introduce 'contexts' to make science learning meaningful and purposeful for pupils. A
typical approach to this is to use an everyday scene to contextualise a science
investigation. For example, an investigation was set up to find out how temperature
affected the time taken for sugar to dissolve (Murphy
science concept that the teacher wanted to teach and made relevant through the context of
a family scene drinking tea.
The reactions of a girl and boy were characteristically different. The girl integrated the
context in formulating her response to the task. The boy ignored the context. He went
straight to the task ‘Find out how the time taken for sugar to dissolve depends on the
temperature of the liquid’ and wanted to test a range of temperatures including room
temperature. The girl could see no point in testing cold water. As she commented
"nobody drinks cold tea." Neither the boy who was working as her partner in the
practical task nor the class teacher could understand the girl's perspective. She would not
‘play the game’ as would the boy and as the teacher intended. She saw no point in
investigating anything that was outside the real-world English context of drinking hot tea!
This ‘ritual of science in the classroom’, although for a different purpose, we have called
‘school knowledge’ in the novice teacher example above as this is the approach to
science investigations implicit in what is set by examination boards. The girl's main
problem was that her solution had to be applied in the context of tea drinking. Another
pupil acting as a mediator tried to help Rennie keep her concern with the authenticity of
the context but also to play the science game. "Say Martians came down Rennie, they
might not know about drinking cold tea. They might
supported by this but basically accepted defeat and carried out the (in her view) artificial
task required by the teacher, at some considerable cost in her view of herself in relation to
science and to the teacher.
This concern with the reality suggested by context can be also be confirmed by a further
example which also illustrates the way that boys often react differently to girls (Murphy,
1988). For an investigation of the thermal properties of different textiles such as nylon,
felt, cotton wool and a range of other similar materials, pupils were given a copper can,
hot water, thermometers and a stop clock. The pupils were asked to find out which of the
materials was the best insulator to make a jacket for a mountaineer. In a similar way to
the sugar and tea example, the boys at once saw that what the game to be played was all
about. They set about wrapping the various materials around the can full of hot water
and plotted a range of comparative cooling curves. The girls’ reaction to the context was
different. Some wanted to make a small jacket to do a ‘proper test’ on it and spent a lot
of time making such a model. Some other girls rejected at the outset cotton wool (the
best insulator amongst the samples offered) as ‘No one would make a mountaineer’s
jacket out of cotton wool!’
12
conclusion. Shifts in this have been to include hypothesis as opposed to title, andet al, 1996). ‘Dissolving’ was thelike cold tea!" The girl feltExample: novice teachers in training
This example illustrates the difficulty teachers have in bringing together the different
types of professional knowledge when organising lessons. The theoretical framework
underpinning this work was developed by one of the authors and colleagues in the Centre
for Research and Development in Teacher Education at the Open University (see Moon
and Banks 1996, Banks 1997) and has been explored with many technology teachers (see
Banks & Barlex, 1999, Banks
both very new and still on a pre-service course this example illustrates rather starkly the
dilemmas which still face more experienced teachers (as shown above). The example has
implications not only for how we should conceptualise the teacher training curriculum
and but also lessons to be learnt for better links across school science and technology. As
we will see, despite very obvious overlaps in curriculum content, here there was little
collaboration between the teachers of science and those of technology.
Although they are at the
teachers, Alun and Geoff have already planned and begun to pair-teach a series of lessons
for their placement school. The department was concerned that the existing school
scheme of work offered to 11 year-olds did not yet include aspects of simple electronics.
Although some discussions took place with members of the Science department, the
student teachers were largely left to themselves to carry out this work. Using their own
ideas and curriculum materials such as text books and electronic kits already in the
school, the students decided to organise their teaching around the development of a face
mask with flashing eyes. They found this a very difficult exercise, and as we will see, the
face mask product was rather pushed out by other considerations. A particular lesson
concerned the pupils investigating which materials were conductors and which insulators.
To do this the student teachers employed a standard kit called
about the circuit by drawing diagrams on the chalkboard.
et al. 2004). Although the teachers in this example arebeginning teaching phase of their course to become D&TLocktronics, but talked firstSubject knowledge
The teachers’ own understanding of simple electricity was sufficient, but lacked the
flexible and sophisticated features to ensure that it was conveyed clearly (McDiarmid
al
nature of the topic pertinent to this design and make task. For example: a description
they gave of current flow also involved a confusing discussion of electron flow; a picture
of a battery was combined (incorrectly) with a diagram of the electrical symbols. The
rather unsatisfactory chalk-board illustration shown in Figure 2 was the result, which
inadvertently corresponded to a classic ‘clashing current’ misconception of pupils
(Shipstone, 1985).
13
et., 1989). They understood electricity themselves, but were unsure of the depth andFigure 2: Chalkboard diagram
School knowledge
The purpose of the project was unclear in the minds of the beginning teachers. When
describing the task they would sometimes see it as means to teach designing and making
(a practical ‘Design and Make Assignment’), however the functional aspects of wearing
the mask were not thought through (e.g. the weight of the battery, its location, or how it
would be supported). They also considered practical skills such as soldering as being
central, but had not allowed enough time to develop such skills. In practice, the face
mask became a means of ‘selling’ the lesson to the pupils – but that became secondary to
the desire to teach aspects of conceptual knowledge about electric circuits.
7Geoff and Alun thought that an understanding of V=IR was important, but the science
department staff had suggested that the use of such a difficult equation would not be
taught and reinforced by them to these 11-year-old pupils. Although a D&T lesson, their
desire to teach the
insulators and the existence of electrons, cut down on the time for any designing and
making. They were unclear if the overall purpose of the activity was designing, acquiring
specific skills, or a ‘seeing-is-believing’ confirmation of scientific principles. Their prior
selection of the subject knowledge they wished to teach was transposed into knowledge
for teaching but, as their understanding of school technology was poor, it was without the
necessary pedagogic rationale or appropriate teaching strategies.
science subject background, such as (in this lesson) conductors andPedagogical knowledge
Only Geoff had used the electronics kits before as a pupil, and both novice teachers were
unfamiliar with the way they could be used in the classroom. The pupils had some
difficulty in manipulating the components and interpreting the circuits the teachers had
constructed on the boards. Making a simple series circuit with battery and bulb was
difficult enough with the new kits, and introducing a break to accommodate different
7
some electronics (McCormick & Davidson, 1996).
We have found this kind of problem with experienced teachers, where the context is used to ‘deliver’14
shaped rods of various materials in an experiment to classify ‘conductors’ and
‘insulators’ defeated almost all pupils.
As these beginning teachers were not able to enlist the experience of their mentor (whose
own subject was business studies), they drew on their own embryonic pedagogical
knowledge to formulate teaching activities for the project. They naturally used analogies
to try to convey ideas about electrical flow.
easier to walk around a hill, rather than walk over it, in an attempt to quickly cover the
idea of a short circuit. As they considered a knowledge of electrons an essential prerequisite
to an understanding of conductors and insulators, Alun showed the following
real model and then talked about it using this chalk-board diagram (Figure 3).
8 For example, Geoff talked about how it isFigure 3: Model of electrons in a wire
Clear
plastic
tube
Ball
bearing
The actual tube, shown to the pupils later, represented the wire and the ball bearings were
the electrons. It is unclear what the pupils thought about the size of electrons and the
need for a conductor for electron flow!
Geoff and Alun wished to scaffold the learning of the pupils and they believed a handson
approach was appropriate. However, they found it difficult to leave the pupils to
experiment with the kits, and continually intervened to move them on because of time
shortage. Too much was attempted too quickly and some pupils became confused then
bored. The novice teachers did not have the pedagogical knowledge to know which
aspects of electricity were difficult to convey.
Personal subject constructs
Both Geoff and Alun have a personal subject construct molded by experience in industry,
which strongly influences their direction and orientation to how and why pupils should
learn Technology. They both also have views of how pupils learn and what constitutes
8
There is good evidence on the difficulty of many analogies (eg Dupin & Joshua,1989)15
‘good’ teaching. They both see hands-on as being vital (although they get side-tracked by
a view that detailed theoretical science concepts are an inevitable precursor to
understanding of school technology) and wish to emphasise a link to marketing the facemask
product (although that aspect is not made explicit to the pupils).
Alun: I've a belief that everyone should follow Technology with a business and a legal aspect,
i.e. unless you know how much it's gonna cost, it's pointless designing something […] Can we
make it? Far too often we find we design things which do not take into the remit […] realistic
targets. So I'd like to relate Technology to more…creative depth within the curriculum.
(Interview)
We feel that the personal subject construct of teachers, such as articulated by Alun, has
been a crucial factor influencing the way that teachers select the information from the
specified curriculum, chose their teaching strategies and thereby affect pupil learning.
Banks and Barlex (1999) support this view, arguing that, within the designing and
making process there are features that will appeal in different degrees to a teacher
according to the specialism and professional history of that teacher. The list below
identifies such features and the often-articulated rationale for its significance. (see Table
1).
Table 1: possible elements of ‘personal subject constructs’
•
product.
Aesthetics The appearance is crucial. It says everything about the•
accomplished.
Communicating skills Unless they communicate their ideas nothing will be•
can be achieved.
Design procedures Without the procedural competence of design nothing•
Making skills But if they can’t make it it’s a complete waste of time.•
Technical understanding If it’s not technically sound it just won’t work.•
endeavour the whole exercise lacks worth
Ideally a balanced design and make assignment will call on each of these features if not
in equal measure then certainly to a meaningful extent. But, if a teacher is strong in just
one or two aspects, or believes that one is more significant than any of the others, the
breadth and balance within the designing and making experience is lost. Many
technology teachers were trained initially as craft teachers and their work has been
generally criticised by the Office for Standards in Education.
Values Without an appreciation of the values implicit in the.. .in general pupils' attainment in designing lags behind that in making. This is because pupils are
either not introduced to a sufficiently wide range of designing strategies [...] or are not taught to use
them effectively. Pupils are generally confident where work is closely directed by the teacher, but less
so when working independently to their own plans, with little awareness of how their work will
develop in the later stages of their projects.
(OFSTED, 1998)
16
The above quotation points up one of the lessons to be learnt from the tradition in both
science and technology for practical hands-on work. To make the tasks manageable in
the classroom, economic on resources and generally successful in terms of teacherintended
outcomes, teachers tend to closely direct the activity of pupils.
We therefore have the situation where both science and technology teachers who adopt a
general problem-solving approach to investigation and design run the risk of it being
treated as rituals in the classroom. These rituals become associated with the science and
technology classrooms and hence students' problem-solving strategies are more to do
with this classroom culture, than with problem-solving in the domains of science and
technology.
We now turn to consider what pupils are learning in science and technology lessons; how
what is specified by government, mediated into tasks by teachers results in ‘learning’ by
pupils.
The Experienced Curriculum
A limited, but important manifestation of how the curriculum is experienced, in terms of
the outcomes of pupil learning, are the scores that pupils achieve on tests and
examinations. The international tests in science conducted in 1995, 1999 and 2003
(TIMSS 2004) indicate science assessment results for 14 year old pupils in each of the 25
countries which undertook the tests on these occasions. On that measure how do pupils
in England perform? In the 2003 ‘league table’ England came 6
of 544 behind (in rank order) Singapore (578), Taipei (571), South Korea (558), Hong
Kong (556) and Japan. (552). Leaving aside the technicalities of the testing process, and
what features of science it was testing (and the undersized sample for England), this can
be seen as good news to the government and to its curriculum advisers. In terms of
trends too, the average science score of pupils in England rose from 533 in 1995 and 538
in 1999. There are no similar international comparisons for Technology, and less
consensus internationally about what constitutes the common features of school
technology (Banks,1996), however other indicators exist which give clues about the
difficulty of science and technology as subjects and pupils satisfaction with their learning
experience, as we will shortly show.
A second indication of the experienced curriculum is the amount of participation in the
subject, especially where pupils have a choice of what to study. In England the General
Certificate in Secondary Education (GCSE), set by government-recognised examination
boards, is taken by all pupils around the age of 16 years, and seen by many as the ‘school
leaving examination’. However, pupils choose (in conjunction with their teachers) what
particular subjects to study for and, within specific subjects what course to take, all
controlled by the framework of the national curriculum. Those wishing to study in higher
education stay at school for two more years to take Advanced Level (A Level)
examinations, and here we have an even sharper choice, which is likely to link to a
pupil’s future career. By considering the data of both science and technology GCSE
candidates, and comparing them with A level candidates, one can make some rough
conclusions about participation in further study of science and technology, when such
study is no longer compulsory. Data exists for the years 1992 to 2001 from the
17
Qualifications and Curriculum Authority provided by the GCSE awarding bodies in
England, Wales and Northern Ireland, and are for candidates of all ages, although the
majority were 16 at the time of the examinations. All D&T courses are included and the
most common science GCSE ‘Science Double Award’, which includes aspects of all
sciences and takes twice as much time to study as technology, counting as two ‘subjects’
(Table 2).
th with an average scoreTable 2: Number of candidates entered for D&T and Science (Double Award GCSE)
Year Total Candidates GCSE Total Candidates
D&T
courses from 1997)
(inc. ShortTotal Candidates
Science Double
Award
1992 5028554 183606 621177
1993 4968634 154720 668272
1994 5029599 150353 810371
1995 5431625 414436 924462
1996 5525620 283974 937304
1997 5455665 269642 929523
1998 5398332 444330 948498
1999 5501193 465252 960870
2000 5514310 467931 980536
2001 5672767 475106 1001610
These data are the Joint Council's final results after any enquiries about the results have
been completed.
Table 3: Number of candidates taking A level D&T and the Separate Science.
Year Total A level
Candidates
Total A level
Candidates
D&T
Year-2)
(% GCSE inTotal A level
Candidates Biology
GCSE in Year-2)
(%Total A level
Candidates
Chemistry
in Year-2)
(% GCSETotal A level
Candidates
Physics
in Year-2)
1992 731240 9572 48742 42697 41301
1993 734081 10934 47748 40975 38168
1994 732974 11046 (6%) 50851 (8%) 41231 (7%) 36147 (6%)
1995 730415 10659 (7%) 52255 (8%) 42293 (6%) 34802 (5%)
1996 740470 11057 (7%) 52053 (6%) 40418 (5%) 33033 (4%)
1997 777710 11572 (2%) 56706 (6%) 42262 (5%) 33243 (4%)
1998 790035 13220 (5%) 57436 (6%) 41893 (4%) 33769 (4%)
1999 787732 13739 (5%) 55810 (6%) 40920 (4%) 33548 (4%)
2000 774364 14650 (3%) 54650 (6%) 40261 (4%) 31794 (3%)
2001 770995 14909 (3%) 52382 (5%) 38702 (4%) 30802 (3%)
(% GCSE18
Table 2 illustrates the increasing popularity of Double Award Science over the years
compared with the total candidates in the GCSE cohort. Similarly as D&T became
established, the number sitting the examination rose steadily and on the face of it, that
could be considered good news for curriculum planners. Both boys and girls are now
obliged to study aspects of the physical sciences and of control technology for example.
However, in both cases at GCSE there was a degree of obligation and that is no guarantee
that when pupils have more choice they will later (e.g. at A level) opt to study science
and technology in more depth.
Table 3 shows the number of candidates taking D&T and the separate sciences at A level.
This is taken by people of around 18 years old who have opted to stay on in education
after the compulsory years of schooling. The percentage figure shown in brackets from
1994 onwards is the fraction of the former GCSE subject cohort who passed the subject
two years earlier (e.g. 1992), who went on to sit the corresponding examination at A
level. Thus 6% of those who in 1992 passed GCSE in D&T went on to do A level in
D&T; similarly 8% went on to do Biology at A level. Looking at the total number of A
level examination candidates over the years, only D&T is rising in absolute terms.
Biology goes up then down and both Chemistry and Physics have a generally downward
trend. However, when one considers the fraction of the corresponding GCSE cohort who
could
trend since the introduction of compulsory study of science and technology has been that
a smaller fraction wish to do so. Naturally looking at this quantitative data does not give
a full picture. Not all schools offer D&T at Advanced level and Physics Teachers are
often very difficult to recruit, so that even A level (and certainly Double Award Science)
is sometimes taught by teachers without a Physics specialism. However, for whatever
reason, the decline in the sciences and the reduction in the percentage wishing to study
science and engineering in higher education is a concern to the government in England as
it is in most of the western world.
The experience of science and technology within compulsory schooling is not increasing
participation subsequently. As we have seen, the way that both science and D&T is
transformed in schools by the pedagogy used, the need to cater for many pupils’ needs at
once, and the requirements of the assessment process all have an impact on the pupils’
experiences of the two subjects. The final example gives some insights into
learning is experienced.
have studied these subjects further at A level should they have wished to so, thehow theExample: problem solving in the technology classroom
We noted earlier that problem solving is often treated as a ritual. We and our colleagues
have reported a body of empirical evidence that this ritual is the way designing as
problem solving can be enacted by teachers, with a limiting experience for pupils (see
McCormick & Davidson, 1996; McCormick, Murphy & Hennessy, 1994). This ritual we
have already suggested was one of the results of the imposed curriculum. However, it
also reflects the state of our knowledge about classroom problem solving that has found
its way into teachers’ knowledge such that it becomes part of their enactment of the
curriculum. An example of this was evident in a case study of a teacher with 12-13 yearold
pupils working on an electronic badge project based on a ‘face’ with LEDs for eyes.
19
The teacher deliberately did not emphasise the design process; it was not one of his main
aims, and he seemed to view designing as a logical approach rather than as a process that
involved sub-processes to be taught and learnt:
…although I'd like them to understand and use the design process
and I think it's quite a nice framework for them to fit things on
to, I don't think there's a great need to be dogmatic about it and
say you must learn it....the nature of projects leads them through
the design process despite the teacher's bit, going through it
with them in front of the class...
(Teacher interview)
He appeared to see the ‘logical approach’ as a ‘way of working’, and in that sense the
sub-processes were of little significance to him. For him the design process was very
much in the background, not just in this project but in general:
I'm relying rather a lot on a subconscious level of going through
things. Some of them won't do it, some will.
It resembles, therefore, a planning tool, and we had evidence of this ritual being used in
other studies even when there was explicit teaching of the overall process as being made
up of sub-process (McCormick & Davidson, 1996).
The particular view a teacher takes of the design process affects the way tasks are
structured, the kinds of interventions that are made by the teacher, and the assessment of
pupils' work. Not all of these will be consistent either with each other, or with the view
espoused by a teacher, but collectively they will have a profound effect on the pupils'
perceptions and activities (the experienced curriculum). But, whatever view is taken of
designing, there is a tendency to see it as an algorithm to be applied in a variety of
situations.
The teacher involved in a
presented:
electronic badge project began it with the 'Situation' beingA theme park has opened in [place] and it wants to advertise
itself. It plans to sell cheap lapel badges based on cartoon
characters in the park. To make these badges more interesting, a
basic electronic circuit will make something happen on the badge.
This was set within the general title of ‘Festivals’, but the links to the ‘Situation’ were not
discussed, and from then on no further reference was made to festivals. The teacher
continued in the session by asking the pupils to define the ‘Design brief’ and draw up a
spider diagram of ‘Considerations’ (a specification), tasks which all the pupils seemed
familiar with. He did not, however, elaborate on the ‘Situation’ or the ‘Design brief’, nor
invite pupils to discuss them in the context of the planned project.
The three pupils we followed (B, T and D) produced different design briefs that
illustrated how the ‘Situation’ was interpreted by them. B & T interpreted it as a "button
is pressed to light up the eyes", whereas D makes no such inference: "to design and make
a clock badge". Their initial ideas of their personal ‘briefs’ lingered and influenced
future tasks; for example, D continued to talk about a "clock face" for several lessons and
abandoned the idea only when he realised that the electronics would not be like that of a
watch. He also imagines that the battery would resemble that in a watch and was almost
20
incredulous when the teacher showed a comparatively large conventional dry 9-volt
battery that he (rightly) considered too heavy for a lapel badge. The teacher's discussion
with D about this issue indicated that unlike D, he had not entered into the ‘Situation’ and
‘Design brief’ in a meaningful way, but only ritualistically - his ultimate answer to the
problem was to "have a strong pin for the badge", a response D felt dissatisfied with.
Next the teacher gave several tasks relating to drawing the faces for the badge, which
implicitly reflected the sub-processes of 'generating ideas', 'developing a chosen idea' and
'planning the making'. However, this was again done in a ritualistic way as the following
indicates.
At the end of the first session pupils were asked, for homework, to create four cartoon
faces as potential designs for the badge. No parameters were given other than that all
four should fit into the design sheet and that pupils should be 'creative'. As with the
'Situation', 'Design brief' and 'Considerations', this step of producing four designs
appeared to be a standard one and, again, was accepted without question by the pupils.
However, in the next session pupils were asked to re-draw the faces so that they touch the
sides of a fixed drawn square (70x70 mm). The reason for this was not made clear until a
later session. Evidence from the pupils' folders indicates that pupils had to modify their
designs in order to fit these new demands. For example, D had originally drawn a thin
'carrot' character, which he had to distort to make it fat enough for it to touch the sides of
the square. The fact that the creation of several designs is perceived by pupils to be a
ritual, is seen in D's comments to the teacher implying he had in fact already made a final
choice while he is still completing the four drawings.
In our research we elaborated some of the strategies that pupils adopted in response to the
various ways the teachers viewed and enacted the problem-solving process (Murphy and
McCormick, 1997). These strategies certainly do not resemble the “algorithms” of
problem solving that are so often taught.
The first strategy is what we characterised as
culture
classroom, and play to those rules. We saw the teacher setting out rules of the game in
our examples of the ‘enacted curriculum’ above. Examples of pupils seeking this culture
out is contrasted in the experience of two girls (Kathy and Alice) producing a mobile.
Alice wanted to do something that clinks when the wind blows, and so had an idea of
using metal. So, given a restricted choice of material, she chose to cut thick mild steel in
the form of disks about two inches diameter. Because she played the rules of the
classroom, Alice ended up with very sore hands, and took a long time; her endeavor
resulted in a very inappropriate way of creating the effect she wanted. (But she did learn
quite a lot about mild steel, as it turned out.)
Kathy had designed a moon and planets going around it, and wanted some kind of
glinting material. When presented with the choice of material, Kathy in contrast to Alice,
looked elsewhere and saw some aluminium (not available to the class) and asked to use
this. The teacher agreed, and she cut this easily with tin snips. Kathy took this approach
many times throughout the project. She broke the rules of the classroom, knowing what
she could and couldn’t get away with. She experienced different kinds of issues and
problems from Alice, but she was avoiding many technological problems.
21
The second strategy is
project involving a moisture sensor. The teacher in this study defined the task in terms of
making a box in which to put the electronics (the transistor circuit, the bulb or the little
speaker, switch, etc.). This had to be appropriate to the situation of detecting moisture or
lack of it. He taught them to cut the material (styrene) in straight lines with a steel ruler
and a knife because when he said “box”, he had in mind a rectangular box. He also gave
them a jig so that they could put the two edges together at right angles and run the solvent
along to stick the two together. But some pupils wanted curved shaped boxes, which
gave some of them at least three emergent problems. First they had to cut a curved
shape, and pupils asked each other and the teacher how to cut the shape as the steel ruler
method wouldn’t work (the solution was to cut it slowly). Second, a curved profile on
one part of the box required one side to bend to follow the profile, but the styrene they
were given was too thick. The pupils asked the teacher who simply gave her a thinner
gauge of styrene, without any discussion. Third, the pupil did not know how to support
or hold the thinner styrene in place to apply the solvent, and so again asked the teacher.
This time the teacher had to think and was obviously solving a problem himself, but
again he gave the
involve her in his problem solving. All she received was the solution without being
involved in the problem solving. This continually being “given solutions” becomes a
culture of the classroom at the expense of a ‘problem-solving’ culture.
In contrast, we found a teacher in a primary school, who worked with younger children
(10- and 11-year-olds), who was able to create this
interactions with students. When pupils came up with problems, the teacher asked
questions about their problem, or posed alternative solutions (because sometimes students
cannot cope with the questions or provide solutions). Pupils were given more than one
solution, because the teacher was trying to engage students in the problem and the
problem-solving process. Such a teacher has to set up a completely different culture in
the classroom. It takes longer, and it is harder to do, but it is crucial to foster problem
solving.
The final strategy is the
ways (see Hennessy & Murphy [1999] for the literature on collaborative activity and
Murphy & Hennessy [2001] for an analysis of examples of collaboration in technology).
One way is through co-operation. In D&T in England pupils are usually set individual
projects, so they may be working alongside each other on a table or a bench, and they can
co-operate because they are doing similar things; they are not identical, but similar
enough to help each other and share tasks.
The second form of collaboration involved pupils in dividing up the task: “You do this
bit, I’ll do that bit. You’re good at that and I’m good at this.” Some of the learning is lost
in this approach. But at least it is a way of collaborating, because they have to put the
two bits together at some stage, and that has an element of good collaborative problem
solving. The final form of collaboration occurs when pupils have a shared task, and they
can talk about it. This means the design of the task must
collaborate. Designed correctly tasks should require solutions to a problem to be
considered by all students through discussion and decision making.
22
These four strategies of problem solving in the technology classroom differ from the way
problem solving is depicted in the national curriculum, and the way technology educators
normally think about it. Without a sensitivity to pupils’ experience of problem solving
the enacted curriculum will not have the required impact imagined by the teacher.
Problem solving in the science classroom has had no similar exploration, partly because
the focus of any problem solving is on the development of conceptual knowledge not
procedural knowledge (Murphy & McCormick, 1997). This gives some scope for
science teachers to learn from technology teachers, even if it is only to be aware of how
they set can up climates that encourage productive problem solving directed at important
scientific approaches to problems.
problem solving as dealing with classroom. This occurs when students try to work out the rules the teacher sets in theproblem solving as giving and finding a solution, illustrated in aresults of his thinking as a ready-made solution to the pupil and did notproblem-solving culture throughstudent collaboration model, and that happens in a variety ofrequire the students toExample: knowledge in the classroom
We indicated in our discussion of the specified curriculum that science education has
been concerned with conceptual knowledge to a greater extent than in technology.
Science educators, and many science teachers, recognise the learning issues involved in
concept development (e.g. as illustrated in CLISP; see Note 3). Despite this concern
there is evidence in technology classrooms that the science knowledge is inert, i.e. it
cannot be used in the technological context. One technology teachers strategy is to
enable this use is to teach knowledge on a ‘need to know’ basis, i.e. when it is needed
within a project. This is problematic, and they under-rate the difficulties for pupils in
learning and using knowledge, as we suggested was the case for the novice teachers
Geoff and Alun in our first example.
One strategy is to design appropriate ‘Focused Tasks’ to cover the necessary conceptual
knowledge requirements. However, teaching knowledge on a ‘need to know’ basis is an
attractive alternative for a technology teacher in the situation where separate 'theory'
lessons would destroy the motivation that the subject is able to engender in pupils. In
addition, the knowledge demands are not always predictable, and hence have to be dealt
with as required. If we consider the electronic badge project discussed in the problemsolving
example above, then it is evident that the teacher would be faced with a variety of
kinds of knowledge, much of which would not feature in the science curriculum for that
year group, or at the very least contains different assumptions about starting points and
progression of conceptual understanding. More to the point, science educators would be
aware of the conceptual difficulties that pupils are likely to encounter, and in particular
the importance of an awareness of alternative frameworks that pupils bring to the lessons.
However, technology teachers are faced with a more complex situation than the carefully
controlled science lesson, where the conceptual knowledge may be used to structure the
tasks. Instead they will have the complexities of knowledge in action and an agenda of
technological knowledge
Levinson, Murphy & McCormick (1997) indicate such problems in a detailed study of
12-13 year-old pupils of the same age involved in a moisture senor project. This revealed
that the science knowledge (in terms of what was learned in the science classroom) was
not available for the pupils to use in their technology activity. In the science classroom
the focus is on
could give an explanation of current flow in simple ‘science circuit’, they could not use it
23
to design something nor to make a circuit work in a particular way.
All of the above knowledge relates to science concepts, but, as noted, to add to the
complexity of pupils' understanding they also have to master technological concepts. In
electronic circuits, and particularly where there is a design element, control system
concepts are used and must be understood by pupils. At this level pupils are introduced
to the idea of
project translates into the light-dependent resistor (LDR) as
associated resistors) as
description and component-level (e.g. an LED) is not without its complications and
arbitrariness (e.g. is the LED's protective resistor part of the output or the process?), and
this became evident in the pupils’ discussions we have researched (McCormick &
Murphy, 1994). In the third session of the project the teacher asked the pupils to make
the match of system descriptors and components having defined
in addition to that of the scientific knowledge.explanations of phenomena, not on its use. Thus, even though studentsinput, process and output, which in the case of the earlier electronic badgeinput, the transistor (andprocess and the LED as output. This match of system-levelinput, process andoutput
out of the LED, giving a clue to the input and output respectively. Nevertheless it took
even the most able pupils, some time to work this out, and more typically a pupil would
insist, quite understandably, that the battery was the input. Indeed this is a legitimate
idea, when
teacher does deal with the idea of the 'transistor as a switch' i.e. as the
any detail. In more recent work (Murphy
olds), we found pupils still having problems with these basic system concepts, and
teachers with different approaches to the underlying control ideas (e.g. no distinction
between open- and closed-loop control; some using the concept of ‘feedback’, some not).
The earlier problems with science concepts are at least well researched, and teaching
strategies exist to deal with some of them, but in the realm of technological concepts we
have much less understanding. Neither do we have much about the interaction of the
different kinds of knowledge required in the technological task. So once again we have
an area where we are unable as teachers to be aware of the experience of the curriculum
for the pupil, without more understanding.
. Pupils were able to use the circuit diagram, which had arrows into the LDR andprimary and secondary inputs are considered (see McCormick, 2004). Theprocess but not inet al, 2004), observing older pupils (14-15 yearConclusion
What lessons can be drawn from a consideration of science and technology education in
England over the years 1984 to 2004? We have covered some issues in passing and here
attempt to draw together what we consider are the crucial points. Although the
framework we have adopted (Figure 1) helps us to focus on specific issues, its
dimensions are naturally interlinked. Classrooms are social environments and the
specified curriculum leads to what is enacted by teachers and what is experienced by
pupils. Yet how pupils react to tasks set and how they learn modifies what teachers do
and, particularly in the early years under consideration, leads to modification of what is
specified.
24
The Specified Curriculum
•
specification in law of what pupils should know.
It is very difficult to control the intended learning of pupils by an elaborateA curriculum specified as a legal document is open to challenge in the court if it is not
carried out in schools. If teachers themselves are not part of the discussion on what
science and technology in school should be, they will ‘teach to the test’ to cover
themselves leading to pedagogies that have, for example, elements of ‘ritual’. There will
be a clash between their personal view of their subject and that specified by the state and
classroom practice will go through a period of extremes until some commonly shared
beliefs of what constitutes ‘good’ teaching emerge. In 2005, this concern to control
centrally the work of teachers has not diminished. Following on nation-wide initiatives
for numeracy and literacy, all teachers of science and of technology will be trained to
improve the learning of 11-14year old – the so called Key Stage 3 Strategy (DfES, 2005).
In countries such as Scotland where the curriculum is suggested by guidelines rather than
legislation, development of the curriculum has been less hectic (see for example Dakers
& Doherty, 2003)
The Enacted Curriculum
•
manageable in the classroom, teachers tend to closely direct the activity of pupils.
In an effort to direct the learning outcomes for all pupils and make the tasksThrough constraints of time and resources, teachers transfer their subject into ‘School
Knowledge’ and pupils play the game of discovering what that is. Some pupils never
quite understand the rules of the game and the relevance of the subject becomes lost to
them; others pick up incidental aspects because teachers have either not made clear what
is salient or their classroom culture produces effects at odds with their rhetoric.
The Experienced Curriculum
•
general satisfaction with the subjects and a desire to study it further.
Requiring the study of the physical sciences and of technology does not lead to•
depends on the view of designing and of investigating held by the teacher.
The way that pupils engage in problem solving in technology and in science•
processes and the science teachers much to teach technology teachers about the
problems associated with acquiring conceptual knowledge.
Technology teachers have much to teach science teachers on the handling ofOur overwhelming conclusion, however, would be that science classrooms and
particularly technology classrooms are under-researched. As new equipment such as ICT
produces yet more pedagogic challenge and new professional development strategies
focusing on its functionality attempted, very little is found out about their impact on the
curriculum experienced by pupils. Despite considerable classroom-based work over the
years 1984-2004 we feel we have merely scratched the surface.
25
26
References
Atkinson, S.: 2000, ‘Does the Need for Higher Levels of Performance Curtail the
Development of Creativity in Design and Technology Project Work?’
Journal of technology and Design Education,
Banks, F.: 1996, ‘Approaches and Models in technology Teachers Education: An
Overview’,
Banks, F.: 1997,
at the PATT-8 Conference, Scheveningen, The Netherlands, April.
Banks, F. & Barlex, D.: 1999, “No one forgets a good teacher! - What do 'Good'
Technology Teachers Know?",
229.
Banks, F., Barlex , D., Jarvinen, E-M, O'Sullivan, G., Owen-Jackson, G. & Rutland, M.:
2004, ‘DEPTH - Developing Professional Thinking for Technology Teachers: An
International Study’,
141-157.
Barlex, D., Black, P & Harrison, G.:1994
Longman, Harlow.
Department of Education and Science [DES]: 1985,
Policy
Dakers, J. R. and Doherty, R.:2003 ‘Technology Education’ in T.G.K. Bryce and
W.M.Humes (Eds)
Edinburgh University Press.
Department for Education and the Welsh Office: 1995,
Curriculum
DfES/QCA: 1999,
DfES:2005
International10(3), 255-281.Journal of Design and Technology Education 1(3), 197-211.Assessing technology teacher professional knowledge. Paper presentedJournal of Design and Technology Education 4(3), 223-International Journal of Technology and Design Education 14(2),Nuffield design and technology: INSET guide,Science 5–16: a Statement of,HMSO, London.Scottish Education, Second Edition Post-Devolution, Edinburgh,Technology in the National, HMSO, London.The National Curriculum, HMSO, London.Key Stage 3 National Strategy, http://www.standards.dfes.gov.uk/keystage3/(accessed January 2005)
Dupin, J.J. & Joshua, S.:1989. ‘Analogies and "Modeling Analogies" in teaching: Some
examples in basic electricity’.
Driver, R. & Oldham, V.: 1986, ‘A constructivist Approach to Curriculum Development
in Science’,
Foulds. K, Gott, R. & Mashiter, J.: 1990,
Garrett, R. M.:1987,. ‘Issues in science education: Problem-solving, creativity and
originality’,.
Gott, R. & Murphy, P. (1987,
Science Education, Hatfield.
Harlen, W. (ed.): 1985,
27
Hennessy, S. & Murphy, P. F.: 1999, ‘The potential for collaborative problem solving in
D&T’,
House of Commons: 2002,
19. Volume 1: Report and Proceedings of the Science and Technology Committee
HMSO, London.
Jenkins, E. (1987, ‘Philosophical flaws’,
Kimbell, R.: 1997,
Levinson, R., Murphy, P. & McCormick, R.: 1997, ‘Science and technology concepts in
a design and technology project: a pilot study’,
Education
Liverpool University: 1994,
McCulloch, G., Jenkins, E. & Layton, D.: 1985,
of School Science and Technology in England and Wales since 1945
Lewes.
McCormick, R.: 1990,
Science Education, 73(2):207-224Studies in Science Education 13, 105-122.Investigations in Science, Blackie, London.International Journal of Science Education, 9 (2), 125-137Assessing Investigations at Ages 13 and 15, Association forPrimary Science: Taking the Plunge, Heinemann, London.International Journal of Technology and Design Education 9(1), 1-36.The House of Commons report: Science Education for14 to,Times Educational Supplement, 2 January.Assessing Technology, Open University Press, Buckingham.Research in Science and Technological15(2), 235-255.SPACE Reports, Liverpool University Press, Liverpool.Technological Revolution? The Politics, The Falmer Press,The Evolution of Current Practice in Technology Education.Paper presented at the NATO Advanced Research Workshop: Integrating Advanced
Technology into Technology Education, 8-12 October, Eindhoven, Netherlands.
McCormick, R.: 1997, ‘Conceptual and procedural knowledge’
Technology and Design Education
McCormick, R.: 1999, ‘Practical knowledge: a view from the snooker table.’ in R.
McCormick & C. Paechter (eds),
London.
McCormick, R.: 2004, ‘Issues of learning and knowledge in technology education’,
International Journal of7(1-2), 141-159.Learning and Knowledge (pp. 112-135), Paul Chapman,International Journal of Technology and Design
McCormick, R. & Davidson, M. (1996, ‘Problem solving and the tyranny of product
outcomes’,
McCormick, R. & Murphy, P.: 1994,
presented at the British Educational Research Association Annual Conference, Oxford
University, England, September.
McCormick, R., Murphy, P. & Davidson, M.: 1994, ‘Design and technology as revelation
and ritual’ in J. S. Smith (ed.),
technology Educational Research and Curriculum Development
Loughborough, Loughborough.
McCormick, R., Murphy, P. & Hennessy, S.: 1994,
technology: a case of situated learning?
American Education Research Association, New Orleans, Louisiana, April.
McCormick, R., Murphy, P., Hennessy, S. & Davidson, M.: 1996, ‘Problem solving in
science and technology education’ Paper presented to American Educational Research
Association Annual Meeting, New York, 8-11 April.
28
McDiarmid, G., Ball, D. L., & Anderson, C. W.: 1989, ‘Why staying one chapter ahead
doesn't really work: subject-specific pedagogy’ in M. C. Reynolds (ed.),
for the Beginning Teacher
Millar, R. & Driver, R.: 1987, ‘Beyond processes’,
62.
Millar, R.: 1988, ‘The pursuit of the impossible’,
Moon, B. & Banks, F.: 1996,
knowledge and pedagogy.
(ATEE), Glasgow, September.
Murphy, P.: 1988, ‘Insights into pupils' responses to practical investigations from the
APU’,
Murphy, P. & McCormick R.: 1997, ‘Problem solving in science and Technology
Education’,
Murphy, P., Lunn, S. A., McCormick, R., Davidson, M. & Jones, H.: 2004,
Evaluation Report. Evaluation of the Promotion of Electronics in Schools Regional Pilot:
Final Report of the Evaluation
Murphy, P., Scanlon, E. & Issroff, K. with Hodgson, B. & Whitelegg, E.: 1996, ‘Group
work in Primary Science - emerging issues for learning and teaching’, in K. Schnack
(ed.),
Educational Studies, Copenhagen.
Office for Standards in Education [OFSTED]: 1998,
Review of Secondary Schools in England
Office for Standards in Education [OFSTED]: 2000,
Design and Technology, 1999-2000,
Qualifications and Curriculum Authority [QCA]: 2005,
2001
Screen, P.: 1988, ‘A case for a process approach: the Warwick experience’,
Education
Shipstone, D.: 1985, ‘Electricity in simple circuits’, in R. Driver, E. Guesene, & A.
Tiberghien (eds.),
Keynes.
Smithers, A. & Robinson, P.: 1992,
Right
TIMSS: 2004, ‘International Student Achievement in Science’ TIMSS & PIRLS
International Study Centre, Lynch School of Education, Boston College, Boston.
Wellington, J.: 1988, ‘Process and content in Physics education’,
14(1), 21-44.Journal of Design and Technology Education 1(3), 230-241.Learning the processes in technology. PaperIDATER 94 – International Conference on Design and(pp 38-42), University ofProblem-solving in design andA paper presented at the Annual Meeting of theKnowledge Base, Pergamon Press, Oxford.Studies in Science Education 14, 33-Physics Education 23(3), 156-159.Secondary school teachers' development: reconceptualisingPaper at Association for Teacher Education in EuropePhysics Education 23, 330-336.Research in Science Education 27(3), 461-481.EiS Final, Open University, Milton Keynes.Studies in Educational Theory and Curriculum, Volume 14, Danish School ofSecondary Education 1993-97: A, The Stationary Office, London.OfSTED Subject Reports SecondaryThe Stationary Office, London.GCSE examination results 1992-http://www.qca.org.uk/7255_2222.html (Accessed January 2005).Physics23(3), 146-149.Children's Ideas in Science (pp. 33-51), Open University Press, MiltonTechnology in the National Curriculum: Getting it, The Engineering Council, London.Physics Education23
Wellington, J. (ed.): 1989,
Routledge, London.
(3), 150-155.Skills and Processes in Science Education: a critical analysis,
No comments:
Post a Comment